Effects of Pedagogical Questioning on Singaporean Young Children’s Learning of Novel Categories

Yun Hui Chwee, Nanyang Technological University

Abstract

There has been a longstanding debate about the advantages and disadvantages of two polarities of teaching methods: direct instruction and discovery learning. Research has shown that questioning might be a viable pedagogical method that combines the advantages of both. When pre-schoolers in the US explored a novel toy with multiple hidden functions, pedagogical questions – questions asked by a knowledgeable teacher who aims to guide children towards learning – have been shown to facilitate more learning and exploration compared to direct instruction or questions asked by a naïve confederate. The current study investigated whether these effects can be observed in Singaporean children’s learning of novel categories. A total of 30 children aged 5–7 (M = 6.51, SD = 0.45) were recruited and randomly assigned to four conditions. In all conditions, children were asked to find out the rule for categorising two types of novel robots by exploring exemplars. Before children started exploring, a hint was given either by a teacher in the form of a direct instruction, by the teacher through a question, by a confederate through a question, or not given. We then measured how much the children explored the exemplars and whether they categorised new cards and identified the rules correctly. Results showed no significant difference between any of the four conditions, which may be due to the small sample size. If a larger sample can confirm the research hypotheses, it will have implications on teachers’ choice of pedagogical methods in early childhood education.

Keywords: Pedagogical questions and learning, adult guidance into exploratory play, target information, enquiry-discovery in exploratory play

Introduction

In educational psychology, there has been a longstanding debate between two polarities of teaching methods: direct instruction (DI) and discovery learning (DL). Proponents of DI suggest that humans learn from direct instructional guidance provided by knowledgeable others, and research has found that it is superior to DL in ensuring efficient and effective learning of target information (Alfieri et al., 2011; Csibra and Gergely, 2009; Kirschner et al., 2006; Klahr and Nigam, 2004; Mayer, 2004). Meanwhile, advocates of DL propose that humans learn best when they discover information by themselves in a minimally guided environment, which develops curiosity and facilitates further learning (Bruner, 1961; Bruner et al., 1976; Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2009; Singer et al., 2006).

As both pedagogies have their advantages and disadvantages, researchers have posited an alternative pedagogy method that combines the benefits of both: Questioning. This teaching method falls under enquiry-discovery, which lies in-between DL and DI, and integrates adult guidance into exploratory play of the student (Dobber et al., 2017; Kriewaldt et al., 2021). Wise and Okey (1983) conducted a meta-analysis regarding the effects of 12 teaching methods on students’ achievement. Teaching techniques included questioning, teacher direction and discovery. Results showed that questioning had the largest effect size on students’ cognitive outcomes. In addition, studies have shown that children explore more when they are faced with conflicting evidence compared to evidence that confirms their hypotheses (Schulz and Bonawitz, 2007; van Schijindel et al., 2015). This suggests that uncertainty between possible hypotheses, which is the essence of questions, may increase children’s self-guided behaviour.

However, different types of questions exist. According to Yu et al. (2018), questions can differ depending on the knowledge state and intention of the questioner. Pedagogical questions (PQ) are questions asked by a knowledgeable person whose intention is to teach the learner. For example, a teacher may ask the child, ‘What does this button on the toy do?’ in an attempt to teach about that particular toy function. On the other hand, naïve questions (NQ) are questions asked by an unknowledgeable person whose intention is to seek an answer from the questionee. An example would be a naïve informant asking the same question, ‘What does this button on the toy do?’ in order to find out the correct answer.

Research has shown that both types of questions elicit different inferences from learners. Based on a pedagogy model, it has been found that human learners interpret differently towards pedagogical and non-pedagogical situations (Shafto et al., 2012; Shafto et al., 2014). In the pedagogical scenario, the learner would infer that the knowledgeable adult has purposefully selected that particular information to be presented, while in the non-pedagogical situation, the learner would infer that the unknowledgeable person has merely chosen a random piece of information. Hence, Yu et al. (2018) predicted children would infer that there is something to be learnt when asked a PQ, but this inference does not occur when they are asked an NQ. The inference is important because it provides an opportunity for children to learn about the target information.

The computational model also provides an explanation for why DI and PQ differ in their effects on learners’ exploration. Yu et al. (2018) elaborated that when children are presented with DI, which is pedagogical, they infer that the teacher has purposefully chosen what to instruct – thus what is not chosen does not need to be considered. Indeed, research has found that DI led to less exploration and further learning (Benefits et al., 2011). Conversely, when children are presented with PQ, Yu et al. (2018) explained that they would question why the teacher chose PQ instead of DI and eventually conclude that there may be more to explore and discover. Thus, Yu et al. (2018) predicted that although both DI and PQ would lead learners to learn about the target information, DI would limit exploration while PQ would encourage children to explore more.

To test out their predictions, Yu et al. (2018) recruited 120 children aged 4 to 5 years and randomly assigned them into four conditions: 1) Direct Instruction (DI), 2) Pedagogical Question (PQ), 3) Pedagogical Question-Overheard (PQO), and 4) Naïve Question-Overheard (NQO). In all conditions, the child is led into a quiet classroom with both an experimenter and a confederate. The experimenter presented the child with a toy and told them that the confederate is unclear about how the toy works. In the DI condition, the experimenter said to the child, ‘Push this button to see what happens’, before pressing a button to demonstrate its function. In the PQ condition, the experimenter asked the child, ‘What happens if you push this button?’ In the PQO condition, the experimenter asked the confederate the same question. Finally, in the NQO condition, the confederate was the one who asked the experimenter the question.

After the prompts were given, the experimenter allowed the child to play with the toy and ended the experiment when the child said he or she was done. The whole procedure was videotaped and coded, and results supported both of their hypotheses. Children in the DI, PQ and PQO conditions were significantly more likely to learn about the target toy function compared to children in the NQO condition. Additionally, children in the PQ and PQO conditions explored the toy and discovered novel toy functions significantly more than children in the DI condition.

Research gap

Existing published studies on PQ only focused on Western children (Jean et al., 2019; Yu et al., 2018). A literature search for similar studies conducted with non-Western children came up with no results. However, past research has found that parenting practices vary across cultures. Unlike their US counterparts, mothers from Japan ask significantly fewer questions of their infants (Bornstein et al., 1992; Toda et al., 1990). Johnston and Wong (2002) also found that compared to Western mothers, Chinese mothers are significantly more likely to believe that children learn best with instructions and less likely to believe that children can learn important information through play. This difference could influence children’s perceptions towards PQ, whereby Chinese and Japanese children are less familiar with their caregivers using questions to teach. Hence, there may be cross-cultural differences when examining the impact of PQ. This current study aimed to recruit Singaporean children, thus filling up this gap in research.

Furthermore, both Yu et al. (2018) and Jean et al. (2019) utilised the novel toy paradigm to measure children’s learning of causal relationships – for example, pressing a particular toy button would cause a yellow tower to light up. There is a dearth of research on the effects of PQ on children’s learning of new categories, which are commonly taught in formal settings – for example, schools teach children about the difference between a living thing and non-living things. Thus, this current study focused on the latter to test the robustness of the positive effects of PQ. Children would be introduced to two types of novel robots who differ by one determining feature and two non-determining features.

Hypotheses

This study had two hypotheses: 1) that children would learn about the determining feature better in the PQ and DI conditions than in the NQ and DL conditions; 2) that children would explore and discover more non-determining features in the PQ condition compared to DI condition.

Method

Research ethics

This study was approved by the institutional review board (IRB) in Nanyang Technological University. Parents were required to fill in a consent form for their child to participate. All the children were given a form to indicate their willingness, using a happy and sad face, to participate as well.

Participants

Thirty children aged 5 to 7 years (M = 6.51, SD = 0.45) were recruited from kindergartens and primary schools in Singapore. They were randomly assigned to one of the four conditions: Pedagogical question (n = 8), direct instruction (n = 8), naïve question (n = 7) and discovery learning (n = 7). Parental consent was acquired before children participated in the experiment.

A photo showing the pedagogical game Planet Zarn
Figure 1: Planet Zarn.
A digital illustration of the pedagogical game Daxes and Wugs
Figure 2: Examples of Daxes and Wugs.
Materials

The materials used in this study were adapted from Williams and Lombrozo (2013). Planet Zarn was created using a laminated green A3-size paper for the base, corrugated boards for the houses, and coloured paper for the decorations (Figure 1). Daxes and Wugs, the two types of alien robots, were shown to be living on opposite sides of the planet. Demonstration cards (n = 32) were created to depict 16 Daxes and 16 Wugs, who vary in three physical features. The determining feature is the feet of the robots, where all Daxes have pointy feet and all Wugs have flat feet (although the exact shape of the feet can differ within each category type). There are two non-determining features: the body shape and colour of the robots. The body shape of Daxes is usually square (75%) and occasionally round (25%), while Wugs are usually round (75%) and occasionally square (25%). In addition, Daxes are usually blue (75%) and occasionally yellow (25%), while Wugs are usually yellow (75%) and occasionally blue (25%).

Table 1 shows the distribution of the different cards:

Daxes Wugs
9 square blue cards 9 round yellow cards
3 square yellow cards 3 round blue cards
3 round blue cards 3 square yellow cards
1 round yellow card 1 square blue card
Total: 16 cards Total: 16 cards
Table 1: Distribution of demonstration cards depicting Daxes and Wugs.

Other than the demonstration cards, there are eight test cards and eight transfer cards. The test cards are the same copies of cards from the demonstration set, while the transfer cards follow the determining and non-determining feature rules but with new feet shapes. The test and transfer cards would consist of four Daxes and four Wugs each.

Procedure

Children were brought into a quiet room in their kindergarten or primary school, and the experimenter asked them by to sit beside a confederate. In all conditions, the experimenter introduced herself as the teacher, and both the child and the confederate as students. Afterwards, the experimenter explained about planet Zarn and the two different types of robots living on it, Daxes and Wugs. The experimenter proceeded to tell the child that his/her task is to figure out the differences between Daxes and Wugs and asked both the child and confederate to guess what the differences were. After guessing, the experimenter followed up with a prompt depending on the experimental condition. In PQ condition, the experimenter asked a question that hinted about the feet (‘Which robot would be more likely to fall over?’) In DI condition, the experimenter explicitly told the child what the determining feature is (‘All Daxes have pointy feet and all Wugs have flat feet!’) In NQ condition, the confederate asked the question instead of the experimenter. In DL condition, no prompt was given.

The experimenter then allowed the child to explore the demonstration set on his or her own to find out the differences between both categories of robots. If the child stopped exploring the cards for more than two consecutive seconds or said he or she was finished, the experimenter asked, ‘Are you done?’ and ended the exploration phase if the child answered ‘Yes’. After the child finished exploring, the experimenter presented him or her with the test and transfer cards and told the child to point towards which house they belonged to (Daxes or Wugs). Finally, the child was asked to verbally state the differences between Daxes and Wugs. For each of the three features, one determining (feet) and two non-determining (shape and colour), the child was given a hint if he or she was unable to state any differences. The whole procedure was videotaped.

A graph showing the number of cards correctly sorted for each condition
Figure 3: Correct sorting of new cards.
Coding

All videos were coded by two research assistants according to an agreed coding scheme (see Appendix). For research hypothesis 1, two outcome measurements were coded: number of test and transfer cards the child correctly sorted, and whether the child correctly recognised the determining feature. For research hypothesis 2, three outcome measurements were coded: length of exploration time, the number of cards explored, and whether the child correctly recognised both of the non-determining features. Inter-coder reliability was high for all measurements (number of test and transfer cards correctly sorted: κ = 1; correct recognition of determining feature: κ = 1; length of exploration time: r  = .98; number of cards explored: r  = .99; correct recognition of non-determining features: κ  = .94 and 1).

Data analysis

All data was entered and analysed in IBM SPSS 25. Planned linear contrasts were used to analyse both research hypotheses. For research hypothesis 1, a planned contrast with +1 weight for both PQ and DI, and -1 weight for both NQ and DL was carried out on the number of test and transfer cards correctly categorised and the recognition of the determining feature. For research hypothesis 2, a planned contrast with +1 weight for PQ and -1 weight for DI was carried out on the length of exploration, the number of cards explored and the recognition of both non-determining features.

A graph showing the rate of recognition of determing feature for each condition
Figure 4: Recognition of determining feature.

Results

Transmission of target information

In contrast with NQ and DL conditions, children in the DI and PQ conditions were predicted to learn about the determining feature that differentiates Daxes and Wugs. Our results did not support this hypothesis (Figures 3 and 4). There was no significant difference observed in the number of test and transfer cards correctly sorted; PQ: 9.75/16, DI: 10.25/16, NQ: 9.29/16, DL: 8.86/16; PQ and DI combined compared to NQ and DL combined, t(26) = .83, p > .05. There was also no significant difference observed in the correct recognition of the determining feature: PQ: 1/2, DI: 1.5/2, NQ:.86/2, DL:.86/2; PQ and DI combined vs NQ and DL combined, t(26) = 1.23, p > .05.

Exploration and further learning

We predicted that children in the PQ conditions would explore and discover more non-determining features than children in the DI condition. However, our results did not support this hypothesis (Figures 5, 6 and 7). There was no significant difference observed in the length of exploration time; PQ: 108s, DI: 134s, t(14) = -.32, p > .05. There was also no significant difference in terms of the number of cards explored; PQ: 11.13/32, DI: 10.63/32, t(14) = .12, p > .05. Lastly, there was no significant difference observed in the recognition of both non-determining features; PQ: 2.75/4, DI: 3.13/4, t(14) = -.67, p > .05.

A graph showing the length of exploration for each condition
Figure 5: Length of exploration time.
A graph showing the number of cards explored for each condition
Figure 6: Number of cards explored.
A graph showing the rate of recognition of non-determing feature for each condition
Figure 7: Recognition of non-determining features.

Discussion

This study examined the effects of PQ on Singaporean children’s learning of new concepts. We hypothesised that 1) children will learn about the determining feature better in the PQ and DI conditions than in the NQ and DL conditions, and 2) children will explore more and discover more non-determining features in the PQ condition compared to the DI condition. Preliminary findings so far with a small sample do not support either hypothesis. Children in all conditions were equally likely to sort out the correct number of cards and recognise the determining feature. Additionally, children in the DI and PQ conditions did not differ in their exploration time, number of cards explored and their recognition of non-determining features.

Our findings are inconsistent with the results found by Yu et al. (2018). A plausible explanation could be that Singaporean children, unlike their Western peers, are unfamiliar with PQ as a pedagogical tool. Past research has found that Chinese and Japanese parents asked significantly fewer questions and believed that children learn best through instruction compared to Western parents (Bornstein et al., 1992; Johnston and Wong, 2002; Toda et al., 1990). This suggests that Chinese and Japanese parents rarely use questions to teach; hence their children may not have learnt to associate questions with teaching at such a young age. When the knowledgeable ‘teacher’ in the PQ condition asks Singaporean children a question, they did not view the question as pedagogical, and they did not infer that the ‘teacher’ intended to teach about the information contained in the question. Without making the inference, the children will not be aware that there is target information to be learnt. This could explain why the question that was supposed to be pedagogical did not have an effect on their learning of the determining feature. However, it is surprising to note that DI did not guarantee children’s learning of the determining feature. Prompting Singaporean children with DI should have ensured that they learnt about the target information, considering that DI is often used in Singaporean culture. The children should have had a strong association between DI and teaching to make the aforementioned inference. Future studies could look into this area to determine the reasons why DI was not effective in this study.

PQ also did not lead Singaporean children in this study to explore more. This may be because they do not view the ‘teacher’s’ question as pedagogical; hence they did not question why the ‘teacher’ chose a question instead of DI to teach. As a result, they will not conclude that there may be more to be explored and learnt.

There are a few limitations in this study. First, the number of cards explored may have been coded slightly inaccurately, despite great attempts to ensure the correct codes. During the exploration phase, some children took out a stack of demonstration cards and flipped through them using the side nearest to the robots’ feet. As the camera was filming the children from the experimenter’s side, it was difficult to discern the correct number of cards the child looked at from the video. This may have influenced the final results shown. Future studies could look into implementing measures to assist coders in counting the correct number of cards the child viewed.

Second, this study used two-dimensional cards, which does not allow for the children to experiment with the attributes of the robots. Hence, the children have to rely solely on sight and abstract thought to figure out the differences. Future studies can look into using three-dimensional toys that children can feel and touch. Those toys would have allowed the children to attempt standing the robots up, thus making it easier for them to find out the differences in feet.

Third, this study used a small sample size. Originally, we aimed for a minimum of 20 children in each condition. However, due to COVID-19 restrictions by the government and participating schools, each of our conditions had fewer than ten children. A small sample size increases the likelihood of a Type 2 error, which occurs when the null hypothesis is incorrectly accepted (Columb and Atkinson, 2016). It is still early to conclude that PQ does not have an effect on Singaporean children’s learning of new concepts. Future studies should target a larger sample size for a reliable final conclusion to be made.

If a larger sample size does confirm both research hypotheses, it would have implications for Singaporean teachers’ choice of pedagogical methods in early childhood education. Singapore’s Ministry of Education (2012) developed a kindergarten curriculum framework titled ‘iteach’, which had the purpose of teaching children through guided play. If PQ was eventually found to be effective, pre-school teachers could incorporate this evidence-based pedagogy method into the current ‘iteach’ curriculum. In the future, studies could also investigate the effects of PQ across different learning domains – for example, mathematics. We believe PQ to be a promising field in pre-school education that could improve on the current pedagogy methods used worldwide.

Conclusion

Our results showed that, in this instance, PQ had no effects on Singaporean children’s learning of new categories in terms of their learning of the target information and their exploration.

It must be emphasised that the small sample size is insufficient in testing both research hypotheses and concluding the effectiveness of PQs. Future analysis using a larger sample size is required and PQ is recommended as a topic for further research, considering its potential implications on early childhood education.


Acknowledgement

I would like to acknowledge the funding support from Nanyang Technological University – URECA Undergraduate Research Programme for this research project.

I (Yun Hui) would like to express my sincere appreciation for my URECA supervisor, Yue Yu, and his full-time research assistant, Jallene Chua Jia En. My completion of this project would not have been possible without their invaluable guide and assistance. I would also like to express my gratitude towards my family and friends for their constant encouragement and belief in me.

List of figures

Figure 1: Planet Zarn.

Figure 2: Examples of Daxes and Wugs.

Figure 3: Correct sorting of new cards.

Figure 4: Recognition of determining feature.

Figure 5: Length of exploration time.

Figure 6: Number of cards explored.

Figure 7: Recognition of non-determining features.

List of tables

Table 1: Distribution of demonstration cards depicting Daxes and Wugs.

Appendix: PQC coding scheme

Exploration phase

Total cards explored

Count the number of cards that the child touches and looks at.

If the child touches a card but does not look at the robot, do not count it as a card explored (e.g. child takes a whole stack at once but only looks at the top card – this is counted as one rather than the number of cards in the stack). However, if the child looks at the card one by one in the stack (even briefly, like fanning it out and seeing part of each card), count all cards in the stack.

If no. of cards coded by both coders are not too far apart (1–2 cards), take the average as the final code.

No. of switches

Count the number of times the child switches from touching one stack to touching the other.

Exploration time

General rule of thumb: The start time is when the child touches the first card spontaneously, and the end time is when the child indicates he or she is finished (turns around to look at the experimenter or says he or she is done).

Testing phase

General rule of thumb:

Feet

Count variations of ‘feet’ (e.g. child says ‘leg’) and variations of ‘pointy’ and ‘flat’ (e.g. child says ‘sharp’ or ‘triangle’ and ‘rectangle’).

Count if the child’s response reflects their understanding of the difference (e.g. child says ‘one can stand, one cannot stand’) even if the specific characteristic was not stated.

Body shape

Count variations of ‘body’ (e.g. child says ‘head’) and variations of shape (e.g. child says ‘circle’ instead of ‘round’).

References

Alfieri, L., P. J. Brooks, N. J. Aldrich and H. R. Tenenbaum (, 2011), ‘Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning?’ Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021017

Bonawitz, E., P. Shafto, H. Gweon, N. D. Goodman, E. Spelke and L. Schulz (2011), ‘The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery’. Cognition, 120(3): 322–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.001

Bornstein, M. H., J. Tal, C. Rahn, C. Z. Galperin, M.-G. Pêcheux, M. Lamour, S. Toda, H. Azuma, M. Ogino and C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (1992), ‘Functional analysis of the contents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Japan, and the United States’. Developmental Psychology, 28(4): 593–603. https://doi.org/10.1037/00121649.28.4.593

Bruner, J. S. (1961), ‘The act of discovery’. Harvard Educational Review, 31: 21–32.

Bruner, J., A. Jolly and K. Sylva (1976), ‘Play: Its role in development and evolution. London: Basic Books.

Columb, M. O. and M. S. Atkinson, (2016), ‘Statistical analysis: Sample size and power estimations’. BJA Education, 16(5): 159–61. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjaed/mkv034

Csibra, G. and G. Gergely, (2009), ‘Natural pedagogy’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4): 148–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005

Dobber, M., R. C. Zwart, M. Tanis and B. van Oers (2017), ‘Literature review: The role of the teacher in inquiry-based education’. Educational Research Review, 22(1): 194–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2017.09.002

Hirsh-Pasek, K., R. M. Golinkoff, L. E. Berk and D. G. Singer, (2009), ‘A Mandate for Playful Learning in Preschool: Presenting the Evidence. Oxford University Press.

Jean, A., E. Daubert, Y. Yu, P. Shafto and E. Bonawitz, (2019, July), ‘Pedagogical questions empower exploration’, in Goel, A. Seifert, C. and Freska, C. (eds.), Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 1: 485–91.

Johnston, J. R. and M. Y. A. Wong (2002), ‘Cultural differences in beliefs and practices concerning talk to children’. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(5): 916–26. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/074)

Kirschner, P. A., J. Sweller and R. E. Clark, (2006), ‘Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching’. Educational Psychologist, 41(2): 75–86. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1

Klahr, D. and M. Nigam. (2004), ‘The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: effect of direct instruction and discovery learning’. Psychological Science, 15(10): 661–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00737.x

Kriewalft, J., L. Robertson, N. Ziebell, R. D. Bias and Clarke, D. (2021), ‘Examining the nature of teacher interactions in a collaborative inquiry-based classroom setting using a Kikan-Shido lens’. International Journal of Educational Research, 108: 101776. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2021.101776

Mayer, R. E. (2004), ‘Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning?: The case for guided methods of instruction’. American Psychologist, 59(1): 14–19. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.14

Ministry of Education. (2012), Nurturing Early Learners: A Curriculum Framework for Kindergartens in Singapore. Singapore: Ministry of Education.

Schulz, L. E. and E. B. Bonawitz, (2007), ‘Serious fun: Preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded’. Developmental Psychology, 43(4): 1045–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.1045

Shafto, P., N. D. Goodman and M. C. Frank, (2012), ‘Learning from others: The consequences of psychological reasoning for human learning’. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(4): 341–51. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612448481

Shafto, P., N. D. Goodman, and T. L. Griffiths (2014), ‘A rational account of pedagogical reasoning: Teaching by, and learning from, examples’. Cognitive Psychology, 71: 55–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2013.12.004

Singer, D. G., R. M. Golinkoff and K. Hirsh-Pasek, (eds.) (2006), Play = Learning: How Play Motivates and Enhances Children's Cognitive and Social-Emotional Growth. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Toda, S., A. Fogel, and M. Kawai (1990), ‘Maternal speech to three-month-old infants in the United States and Japan’. Journal of Child Language, 17(2): 279–94. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900013775

van Schijndel, T. J., I. Visser, B. M. van Bers and M. E. Raijmakers, (2015), ‘Preschoolers perform more informative experiments after observing theory-violating evidence’. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131: 104–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.008

Williams, J. J. and T. Lombrozo (2013), ‘Explanation and prior knowledge interact to guide learning’. Cognitive Psychology, 66(1): 55–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.09.002

Wise, K. C. and J. R. Okey, (1983), ‘A meta-analysis of the effects of various science teaching strategies on achievement’. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 20(5): 419–35. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.3660200506

Yu, Y., A. R. Landrum, E. Bonawitz and P. Shafto (2018), ‘Questioning supports effective transmission of knowledge and increased exploratory learning in pre‐kindergarten children’. Developmental science, 21(6): e12696. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12696

Glossary of Terms

Direct instruction: Teacher-centred approach that involves the teacher providing students with structured information.

Discovery learning: Student-centred approach that involves the students exploring and discovering the solution to a problem by themselves.

Enquiry-discovery: Teaching method that involves guidance by the teacher and active discovery by the student.

Pedagogical model: A model that supports teachers to design engaging and challenging learning experiences through planned integration of curriculum, pedagogy and assessment.

Self-guided behaviour: Behaviours that are guided by oneself.

Type 2 error: Null hypothesis is incorrectly accepted – e.g. A COVID test showed up as negative, even though the person is infected.

‘iteach’: A Singaporean educational framework that included guided play into the teaching curriculum.


To cite this paper please use the following details: Chwee, Y. H. (2023), 'Effects of Pedagogical Questioning on Singaporean Young Children’s Learning of Novel Categories', Reinvention: an International Journal of Undergraduate Research, Volume 16, Issue 2, https://reinventionjournal.org/article/view/1220. Date accessed [insert date]. If you cite this article or use it in any teaching or other related activities please let us know by e-mailing us at Reinventionjournal@warwick.ac.uk.